Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The association between shift work exposure and cognitive impairment among middle-aged and older adults: Results from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA)

  • Durdana Khan ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    drkhan@yorku.ca

    Affiliation School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

  • Heather Edgell,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

  • Michael Rotondi,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

  • Hala Tamim

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Abstract

Background

Shift work, especially rotating and night shift work, has been linked to a wide range of detrimental health outcomes. Occupational factors like shift work and their potential impact on cognitive functions have received little attention, and the evidence is inconclusive. The objective of our study is to explore associations between shift work exposure and cognitive impairment indicators based on comparisons with the normative standards from the Canadian population.

Methods

Cross-sectional analyses were performed using baseline Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging database, including 47,811 middle-aged and older adults (45–85 years). Three derived shift work variables were utilized: ever exposed to shift work, shift work exposure in longest job, and shift work exposure in current job. Four cognitive function tests were utilized, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Tests (immediate and delayed) representing memory domain, and Animal Fluency, and Mental Alteration, representing the executive function domain. All cognitive test scores included in study were normalized and adjusted for the participant’s age, sex, education and language of test administration (English and French), which were then compared to normative data to create “cognitive impairment’ variables. Unadjusted and adjusted multivariable logistic regression models were used to determine associations between shift work variables and cognitive impairment individually (memory and executive function domains), and also for overall cognitive impairment.

Result

Overall, one in every five individuals (21%) reported having been exposed to some kind of shift work during their jobs. Exposure to night shift work (both current and longest job) was associated with overall cognitive impairment. In terms of domain-based measures, night shift work (longest job) was associated with memory function impairment, and those exposed to rotating shift work (both current and longest job) showed impairment on executive function measures, when compared to daytime workers.

Conclusion

This study suggests disruption to the circadian rhythm, due to shift work has negative impact on cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults and this warrants further investigation.

Introduction

Cognitive impairment is a condition that indicates the transitional phase between normal cognitive function and dementia [1]. As the global population is aging, cognitive impairment has become a major health concern affecting independence and quality of life. Although it was believed that cognitive decline was an inevitable feature of the normal aging process [2], evidence now reveals that cognitive functions are modifiable across the lifespan [3]. A comprehensive meta-analysis of 247 studies examined key risk factors such as socio-economic and behavioural factors for cognitive impairment including dementia and found that lower educational attainment and smoking were strong indicators of cognitive disorders [4]. Similarly, lack of availability of social support has been linked to poor cognitive function among middle-aged and older adults [5].

Little attention has been paid to occupational factors like shift work (SW) and their potential role in cognitive performances. In general, SW refers to a work schedule that occurs outside the traditional daytime (9:00 a.m. to 5:00 p.m.) working hours [68]. The entire spectrum of SW comprises the following shifts: evening shift, night shift, rotating shift (day to evening and/or night), and other less defined shifts including on-call or casual shift (no set schedule) and irregular shifts [7, 912]. A variety of negative health outcomes have been associated with SW, particularly night and rotating SW [9, 10, 1317]. A meta-analysis of 34 studies showed that both types of SW, night and rotating, were associated with an increased risk of coronary events including myocardial infarction and ischaemic stroke; the highest point estimate was noted for night shifts (RR = 1.41, 95% CI, 1.13–1.76) [10]. SW, specifically night and rotating shifts, has been identified as a risk factor for peptic ulcer when compared with regular day workers [13]. A Danish prospective study that followed a cohort of 20,000 nurses for 15 years concluded that night shift workers were at increased risk of developing type-2 diabetes when compared with their day shift counterparts [14]. A previous study investigated female shift workers and have suggested a connection between rotating SW and a delayed menopause [9]. Moreover, the International Agency for Research on Cancer (IARC) has classified night SW as a probable human carcinogen (Group 2A) [18, 19] with evidence of an association with prostate (RR = 1.24, 95% CI, 1.05–1.46) [15], colorectal (OR = 1.32, 95% CI, 1.12–1.55) [16], and breast (HR = 2.15, 95% CI, 1.23–3.73) [17] cancers.

Taken together, these studies support the concept that SW impacts worker’s health significantly. While little is known about the physiological pathways underlying SW-related disease processes, several mechanisms have been hypothesized including circadian misalignment due to disturbed sleep, and light induced suppression of melatonin levels at night [20, 21]. These factors, in turn, disrupt a number of physiological and behavioral processes that contribute to disease progression. Any interference in regular circadian rhythm, due to sleep restriction, could result in disturbed metabolic, hormonal and inflammatory responses [22]. Such misalignment has been found to disturb cortisol levels, and pro- and anti-inflammatory proteins [23], contributing to the development of chronic diseases including cognitive impairment. Another mechanism is endogenous melatonin rhythm. Melatonin is a hormone secreted from the Pineal body, low during the daylight hours and at the highest levels during dark periods (night) [20, 22, 24]. Exposure to light at night can reduce circulating melatonin levels [25]. If the light is bright, the levels can be completely suppressed, which may be a potential risk factor for chronic illnesses [26]. Shift workers experience substantial misalignment between circadian system of the body and unusual working schedules [27], which exposes them to increased risk for health problems. One more important factor that contributes to circadian interruption among shift workers is their behavior/lifestyle. This includes eating at irregular timings, lower physical activity levels, and higher incidence of smoking and intake of alcohol [28]. These factors feed back into the circadian clock causing desynchronized rhythms and altered metabolic and body temperature cycles [20, 27, 29]. In short, the potential effects of SW on cognition are likely related to the circadian misalignment and melatonin suppression. However, it is possible that any or all of the mechanisms described above interact to influence shift worker’s cognitive function [27, 28].

The existing body of literature supports the notion that SW plays a critical role in cognitive function impairments. For example, some studies have highlighted acute and short term negative effects of night SW and disturbed sleep on cognitive functions [3032]. In addition, few studies have assessed consequences of repeated disruption of circadian rhythms on cognitive functioning, among shift workers over time. First population based study [33] that explored chronic consequences of SW on cognitive functioning utilized large cross-sectional sample (3,237 French workers of aged 32, 42, 52, and 62 years) from VISAT (Aging, Health and Work) cohort. SW were classified into ‘current’, ‘former’ and ‘never’ and cognitive performances were evaluated by measuring verbal memory and speed performances. This first work provides some evidence of adverse effects of SW exposure on cognitive functioning, especially for men who are current or past shift workers [33]. Later, a prospective cohort study utilized the same French VISAT data base and reported that rotating SW was linked to lower cognitive test scores (memory and speed performances) [34]. According to the study, former shift workers who stopped working shifts within the previous five years showed improved cognitive functioning [34]. Furthermore, a cross-sectional Swedish EpiHealth cohort study (7,143 participants, aged between 45–75 years) investigated associations between history of SW (non-shift, past and current shift worker) and cognitive executive functions i.e., trail making test (TMT). The study revealed that current and former shift workers performed worse on TMT outcomes than non-shift workers [35]. More recently, analysis of 21,610 participants of aged 45–85 years, from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA) examined the cross-sectional relationship between SW (yes/no) and aspects of cognitive performance (declarative memory and executive functioning) [36]. The study found that shift workers showed poorer cognitive scores on tests for executive functioning (mental alteration test and interference condition of stroop test) but not for declarative memory (immediate and delayed recall trial) compared to non-shift workers [36]. Contrary to the results previously described, there are studies [37, 38] that did not find significant associations between SW and cognitive function. A sample of 595 participants with no dementia from a Swedish Adoption Twin Study of Aging (SATSA), were followed for 9 years [37]. No significant associations were reported between any types of SW (ever/never) and measures of cognitive performance (verbal, spatial, memory, processing speed, and general cognitive ability) [37]. Similarly, The Nurse’s Health Study [38] followed 16,190 female participants in the United States, aged 30–55 years, over a 6-year period with three repeated cognitive assessments. Overall, this study does not convincingly support the hypothesis that older persons’ cognition is negatively affected by their midlife SW history [38]. These discrepancies could be attributed to lack of differentiation between SW schedules (night, rotating) [37], and restriction of study sample to highly educated participants, who held at least a registered nurse or bachelor’s degree [38].

Despite SW’s significance and crucial part in cognitive impairment, there is still a dearth of evidence. Existing normative standards utilized to evaluate cognitive functions were based on non-Canadian samples [3335, 37, 39, 40], which may be outdated [33, 39, 40], and did not cover the full spectrum of ages from middle-aged to older adults [33, 34, 3840]. Most previous studies [3537] did not account for differentiation between SW schedules (night and rotating SW separately) [33, 3538], and educational differences [34] in their analyses. Considering that circadian rhythm regulates cognitive activities [41, 42], desynchronization of the circadian rhythms associated with SW might be one of the plausible mechanisms underlying this association. Given the mixed findings reported from the limited studies on SW in relation to cognitive performance, this study aims to examine the association between SW and cognitive impairment measures based on normative standards from the Canadian population.

Materials and methods

Study design and sample

Cross-sectional data analyses were performed using the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA) database, which is a nationwide, epidemiological study of aging that includes 51,338 middle-aged and older adults (aged 45–85 years). Participants were recruited and baseline data was collected between 2010 and 2015. The CLSA has two cohorts: the “tracking cohort” (21,241) that provided all information through telephone interviews, and a “comprehensive cohort” (30,097) that provided self-reported health information through in-home personal interviews, also, during a site visit at their local data collection center, they provided data including the entire neuropsychological battery. For this study, we combined both cohorts to increase sample size. CLSA excluded residents of institutions, the three territories, First Nations reserves; those who spoke neither English nor French; fulltime Canadian Armed Forces members; and individuals with overt cognitive impairment. The CLSA study design has been previously described in detail elsewhere [43, 44]. The core CLSA study has been approved by McMaster University Health Integrated Research Ethics Board and by research ethics boards at all collaborating Canadian institutions. The present study is a secondary analysis of fully de-identified CLSA data which has been approved by the York University, Office of Research Ethics (ORE) [STU 2020–123]. As such, additional participant consent for this analysis was not required as all CLSA participants provided informed consent during primary data collection to have their de-identified data used in research.

This study sample was limited to those who self-reported being currently employed or having previously worked. Fig 1 displays a flow diagram outlining the exclusion criteria. Participants were excluded if they reported: never working in any job; working in unspecified schedules; and refused to answer or do not know their working schedules. Finally, 47,811 participants remained in the study sample, which formed the basis of analysis.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging participant flowchart.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289718.g001

Assessment of primary exposure (SW)

The primary exposure of interest ‘SW’ was self-reported and assessed in the CLSA baseline questionnaires. All study participants were asked “Have you ever worked at a job or business? (yes/no). The participants who reported ‘yes’ were asked “Are you currently working at a job or business? (yes/no). Participants who reported ‘yes’ were asked “Which of the following best describes your working schedule? (daytime work, night shift, rotating shift). Participants were also asked about their longest job, “Thinking about the job you worked at the longest, which of the following best describes your working schedule? (daytime work, night shift, rotating shift). Based on this information, three variables were generated to measure SW exposure [9]:

Ever exposed to SW.

This variable measured overall occurrence of any SW. All participants who reported ever worked in any shift (night/rotating) in their working career were considered exposed to SW and coded as ‘yes’.

All those who reported only daytime work, were considered unexposed to SW and coded as ‘no’.

Exposure of SW in longest job.

This variable measured the exposure of SW during the longest job. SW in longest job was categorized into; daytime work (reference category), night SW and rotating SW.

Exposure of SW in current job.

This variable measured the exposure of SW among participants who reported currently working and categorized into; daytime work (reference category), night SW, rotating SW.

Primary outcome: Cognitive impairment

The primary outcome for this study is ‘cognitive impairment’, based on four cognitive function tests, including Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Tests (REYI and REYII) representing memory domain, with Animal Fluency (AF2), and Mental Alteration (MAT), representing the executive function domain of cognition. Details are summarized in Table 1. These domains were selected because they are present in both CLSA cohorts and each has been shown to correlate with everyday functioning (i.e. physical, behavioral, and social) [4547]. The CLSA working group created normalized scores from the original test scores as standardized z-scores that have a mean of zero and standard deviation of 1.0. All normed z-scores were adjusted for age, sex and education status, and norming is done separately for tests completed in English and French. In order to determine whether a person’s performance is within the range of healthy cognitive performance, comparisons were made with normative data. Normative data were created by CLSA [48] and used as a comparison standard for an individual’s performance.

thumbnail
Table 1. Description of cognitive function tests utilized by CLSA to create cognitive impairment variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289718.t001

Individual cognitive impairment variables.

Four binary-valued impairment variables (yes/no) were created, one for each of the cognitive function test i.e. REYI, REYII, AF2, and MAT. ‘Yes’ indicates that the participant’s normed z-score falls in the lowest 5% of the neuro-healthy CLSA normative data.

Overall cognitive impairment variable.

A binary-valued variable (yes/no) that indicates the participant’s overall cognitive performance on the basis of four individual cognitive impairment variables and can only be calculated when all four are available i.e. REYI, REYII, AF2, and MAT [48, 49]. The presence of two or more individual impaired scores are suggestive of overall cognitive impairment and coded as ‘yes’, whereas zero or one impaired score is suggestive of ‘no’ impairment overall [48, 49].

Detailed rationale for the selection of these measures of ‘cognitive impairment’ along with explanation of their implementation and validity of utilized base rate algorithms have been published elsewhere [48, 5054].

Potential predictors

Multivariable models were adjusted for a relevant set of confounders. Potential confounders were determined by consulting the existing literature on SW and cognitive impairment [3538]. Sociodemographic variables included, sex, age in years, ethnicity, education level, marital status, total annual household income in Canadian dollars, retirement status [3538]. Lifestyle factors comprised of smoking status and alcohol consumption. Height and weight were used to calculate body mass index (BMI) in kg/m2 [3538]. Depression was ascertained by utilizing The Center for Epidemiological Studies short Depression scale (CES-D10). A score ≥10 suggests the presence of depression [55, 56]. The measure of multi-morbidity was based on the standard definition [57] and included following chronic conditions: anxiety or mood disorder, Alzheimer’s disease, arthritis, asthma, cancer, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, diabetes, cardiovascular disease, and stroke. These chronic conditions were measured in the CLSA using the self-reported question, “Has a doctor ever told you that you have…?” The presence of ≥2 chronic diseases were suggestive of multi-morbidity. CLSA measured Social Support Availability (SSA) by asking participants to rate their level of perceived support [58]. The measure contains 19 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 (none of the time) to 5 (all of the time), with higher responses indicating better perceived support. Finally, the average SSA scores were used to create three categories, low, medium, and high [5]. ‘Type of study cohort’ is included as a covariate to represent the cohort membership, categorized into ‘tracking’ and ‘comprehensive’ [59]. Data from participants in the tracking cohort were collected over the phone, whereas data from participants in the comprehensive cohort were collected in-person [51]. All covariates were measured at baseline and their respective categories are summarized in Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Baseline characteristics of study sample and proportion of cognitive impairment indicators.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289718.t002

Analysis

Baseline characteristics were presented as frequencies. Unadjusted and adjusted logistic regression models were used to determine associations between SW variables and cognitive impairment individually, and also for overall cognitive impairment. Separate models were generated for each outcome variable. Odds ratios (ORs) with 95% confidence interval (CI) were calculated for all models. The CLSA provides survey inflation weights (i.e., inverse probability weights) and analytical weights, which were used for prevalence estimates and regression modeling respectively to generalize results to the Canadian population [60]. A P value less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant. To assess the robustness of our findings, sensitivity analyses were performed that compared complete cases included in analysis to the excluded cases due to missing information related to SW schedules, as well as cognitive impairment. Results from both sensitivity analyses are available in S1 Appendix. All statistical data analyses were performed using STATA version 13.0.

Results

Fig 1 depicts a flow diagram summarizing the criteria used for exclusions. We compared the baseline characteristics of participants excluded due to missing information related to SW schedules (N = 1,682) with the participants included in our final study population (N = 47,811). No significant differences related to cognitive impairment variables were found between two groups. However, the participants excluded tend to be male, older (65 plus), less educated, non consumer of alcohol, and belongs to comprehensive cohort. Details are available in S1 Appendix. We also compared the participants with missing information related to cognitive impairment (N = 1,201) to the participants with complete cognitive impairment data (N = 46,610). No significant differences were found related to SW exposure between two groups. However, the participants with missing information tend to be in older group (65 plus), retired and living without partner, belong to tracking cohort and low income group, consume less alcohol, have depression and multi morbidity. Details are available in S1 Appendix.

Table 2 shows summary statistics of 47,811 participants (weighted to represent 12,632,907 Canadians) at baseline. The mean age of participants was 59.7 years (SD, 10.15 years), and 51.4% were females. Most participants (95%) were White, more than 50% reported to be living with partners, having education of high school to some college level, former smokers, drinking at least weekly, still working (not retired) and had household income 50,000 CAD and more. Around 30% were obese, had multiple-morbidity, and reported low to medium SSA. Overall, one in every five individuals (21.1%) reported having been exposed to some kind of SW at work. 4% and 11.6% of currently working participants, respectively, reported being exposed to night and rotating SW. Considering the longest job held in their whole career, 3.9% and 15.6% of individuals reported being exposed to night and rotating SW, respectively.

The proportions for cognitive impairment (both individual and overall) are included in Table 2. Higher cognitive impairment was noted among those who reported ever exposed to any type of SW compared to those never exposed (daytime work only). Consistently, individuals who reported being exposed to night and rotating SW during current or longest job had a greater proportion of cognitive impairment compared to those who only reported daytime work. Unadjusted logistic regression analyses indicated that SW exposures were associated with higher odds of cognitive impairment compared to those who were unexposed (Table 3). Results of multivariate analyses suggested that SW exposure was related to increased odds of cognitive impairment, after adjustments for confounders (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 3. Unadjusted logistic regression [odds ratios (ORs) and 95% Confidence Intervals(CI)] for individual and overall cognitive impairment for primary SW variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289718.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Adjusted logistic regression [odds ratios (ORs) and 95% Confidence Intervals(CI)] for individual and overall cognitive impairment for primary SW variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289718.t004

Relationship between SW exposure and overall cognition impairment

Separate models were constructed to evaluate the associations between SW exposures and overall cognitive impairment. For ever exposed to SW, no significant associations was observed for overall impairment (OR, 1.12; 95% CI, 0.92–1.35). However, overall cognitive impairment was found significant among participants who reported to be exposed to night SW during their current job (OR, 1.79; 95% CI, 1.08–2.96) and, night SW during their longest job (OR, 1.53; 95% CI, 1.04–2.26) when compared to those who only reported day time work (Table 4).

Results of the multivariate analysis also exposed significant associations among employed participants between sociodemographic factors and overall cognitive impairment (estimators for confounders are not shown in Table 4, details are available in S2 Appendix). In general, overall cognitive impairment was significantly higher among non-white workers (OR, 4.83; 95% CI, 3.55–6.57), workers having depression (OR, 1.80; 95% CI, 1.38–2.34) and having education of high school to some college (OR, 2.37; 95% CI, 1.69–0.83). However, lower odds of overall cognitive impairment were noted for those workers who belong to older age groups (55 and plus) (OR, 0.65; 95% CI, 0.51–0.83), higher income groups (50,000 CAD and above) (OR, 0.48; 95% CI, 0.37–0.63), having some social support (OR, 0.81; 95% CI, 0.67–0.97), and drinks at least weekly (OR, 0.74; 95% CI, 0.59–0.91). Details of complete models including estimators for all confounders are presented in separate tables (S2 Appendix).

Relationship between SW exposure and memory function of cognition

The associations between SW exposures and memory function of cognitive impairment (REYI and REYII) were examined and separate models were constructed. For REYI measure of memory function, no statistically significant results were found across all primary SW variables (Table 4). Based on REYII measure of memory function (Table 4), participants who were exposed to night SW during their longest job were found significantly associated with cognitive impairment compared to those reported only daytime work (OR, 1.44; 95% CI, 1.03–2.01). The association remained non-significant for current night (OR, 1.28; 95% CI, 0.81–2.05) and rotating (OR, 1.03; 95% CI, 0.77–1.36) shift workers.

In addition, the multivariate analysis revealed substantial associations between sociodemographic characteristics and memory function impairment among employed participants (estimators for confounders are not shown in Table 4, details are available in S2 Appendix). Memory function impairment (both REYI and REYII) was significantly higher among non-white workers (OR, 2.12; 95% CI, 1.57–2.86), and workers having depression (OR, 1.31; 95% CI, 1.13–1.52). However, lower odds of memory function impairment (both REYI and REYII) were noted for those workers who belong to older age groups (OR, 0.46; 95% CI, 0.35–0.60), and higher income groups (20,000 CAD and above) (OR, 0.57; 95% CI, 0.44–0.73). REYI impairment reduced for those having social support (OR, 0.81; 95% CI, 0.70–0.94), having high education (OR, 0.74; 95% CI, 0.58–0.93) and those who drink at least weekly (OR, 0.75; 95% CI, 0.63–0.89). Details of complete models including estimators for all confounders are presented in separate tables (S2 Appendix).

Relationship between SW exposure and executive function of cognition

Separate models were generated to evaluate the relationships between SW exposures and executive function impairment (AF2 and MAT). For AF2 measure of executive function, no statistically significant results were noticed across all primary SW variables (Table 4). However, based on MAT measure of executive function (Table 4), cognitive impairment was associated with participants who reported ever exposed to any type of SW (OR, 1.14; 95% CI, 1.00–1.30), exposed to rotating SW in their current job (OR, 1.36; 95% CI, 1.06–1.74), and exposed to rotating SW in their longest job (OR, 1.16; 95% CI, 1.01–1.34) compared to those who had never been exposed to SW (daytime work only). For the AF2 measure of executive function, no statistically significant results were found across all primary SW variables (Table 4).

Furthermore, among participants who were employed, the multivariate analysis showed significant associations between sociodemographic factors and executive function impairment (estimators for confounders are not shown in Table 4, details are available in S2 Appendix). Executive function impairment (both AF2 and MAT) was significantly higher among non-white workers (OR, 3.06; 95% CI, 2.39–3.93), and workers having depression (OR, 1.34; 95% CI, 1.10–1.50). However, lower odds of executive function impairment (both AF2 and MAT) were noted for those workers who belong to older age groups (OR, 0.76; 95% CI, 0.64–0.91), and higher income groups (20,000 CAD and above) (OR, 0.57; 95% CI, 0.34–0.98). Also, MAT based cognitive impairment was found significantly higher for workers who are current smokers (OR, 1.34; 95% CI, 1.10–1.50), and obese (OR, 1.35; 95% CI, 1.08–1.68). Details of complete models including estimators for all confounders are presented in separate tables (S2 Appendix).

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the associations between SW exposure and cognitive impairment among middle-aged and older adults. The results of this study demonstrated that SW exposure has significant relationship with cognitive impairment. Overall cognitive impairment was evident for those exposed to night SW, both during current and longest job, compared to those who worked daytime shift only. In terms of domain-based measures, night SW exposure in longest job was related to memory function impairment and those exposed to rotating SW, both in current and longest job, were more likely than daytime workers to have impaired executive function. These findings are clinically relevant and support the notion that circadian misalignment would render shift workers more vulnerable to cognitive impairment. It is imperative to identify and comprehend modifying risk factors, like SW, associated with cognitive impairment, since this is critical for designing and implementing suitable prevention strategies.

Globally, SW is prevalent and these study results are consistent with the literature [79] indicating that 21% of Canadians were exposed to some kind of SW during their career. Findings from other developed economies such as France [61], Japan [62], across Europe [63] and the United States [63], also confirms similar pattern where 20% to 25% of workers were exposed to SW in various sectors. Moreover, this study reported the overall proportion of cognitive impairment as 4.4%, which is consistent with rates observed (5.3% and 2.8%) in some previous studies [64, 65]. However, other studies [6668], have discovered higher rates (10.8%, 7%, 8.7%) of cognitive impairment. A major factor that may have influenced these differences is the CLSA study design, which included only persons without overt cognitive impairment at baseline [49].

The study results suggest that the association between night SW (both in current and longest job) and overall cognitive impairment were significant. As far as domain-based measures are concerned, night SW (in longest job) and rotating SW (both in current and longest job) were more likely than daytime workers to have impaired memory (REYII) and executive function (MAT) respectively. These findings support evidence from previous research linking SW with cognitive impairment. A population based study [33] that explored chronic consequences of SW (current, former or never) reported that cognitive functions among former shift workers tends to be impaired. Later, a prospective cohort study also stated that rotating SW was associated with lower cognitive test scores [34]. Similarly, a cross-sectional Swedish study linked a history of any SW to lower cognitive performances and observed that current shift workers performed worse on the cognitive tests than non-shift workers [35]. Contrary to the results previously described [3335] other studies [37, 38] did not find significant associations between SW and cognitive function. Possible explanations for these contrasting results might be due to differences in the classification of SW (24), [36] (unable to account for types of SW), age categorization [38] (did not cover full spectrum and limited to age group 58–68 years), and the use of a non-representative highly educated sample [38] (restricted to nurses).

Recently, Alonzo et al. [36] documented lower performance on measures of executive function (MAT) among shift workers. However, the measures based on declarative memory (REYI and REYII) did not find any statistically significant results, whereas our adjusted analysis indicated that exposure to night SW during longest job is associated with impaired measure of memory function (REYII). The results of Alonzo et al. [36] need to be interpreted with caution as several limitations have been identified. First, measures used to assess the cognitive function were based on scores without any demographic adjustments of age, sex, education and language. Exploration of cognition in an aging population without adjustment for demographic variables associated with healthy aging will produce misleading results due to measurement bias [48, 69]. In contrast, our study utilized ‘normalized cognitive scores’ with respect to participant’s age, sex, education and within each language group (English and French), hence reducing the chance of measurement bias [48]. Second, findings lacked comparisons with normative data, which is fundamental for the interpretation of neuropsychological test scores and determining whether a person’s performance is below the range of healthy cognitive performance. Finally, their study [36] did not examine types of SW separately, i.e., night and rotating SW. In contrast, our analysis takes into account different types of SW (night and rotating SW) for both current and longest job. The relationship between SW and cognitive functions may not be same across different types of SW (night and rotating SW). Also, rotating SW has been hypothesized to be more disruptive to circadian rhythm than regular night work and it is possible that rotating shift workers demonstrate greater difficulty in adapting to work schedules as they have to move from shift to shift compared with regular night shift workers [70].

Although the relationship between SW and cognitive performances is inconclusive [3] there are good reasons to believe that such a relationship may exist. One possible pathophysiological mechanism underlying the association between SW, and cognitive impairment has been thought to be the repeated desynchronization of body clock due to working and sleeping at the wrong circadian phase among shift workers [34, 71, 72]. This could demonstrate harmful impacts on health, such as sleep deprivation, daytime sleepiness, and brain inflammation, making people more susceptible to cognitive decline [33, 38, 71, 73, 74]. Another mechanism is the repeated physiological stress and increased levels of cortisol induced by circadian disruption, as evidenced by a previous study [75] which explored the influence of chronic jet lag on cognitive functions among airline cabin crew. Moreover, disturbed circadian rhythms have also been linked to neurodegeneration [76, 77]. It is possible that impaired pineal secretion of melatonin, due to unusual light exposures among shift workers, may significantly impair the normal antioxidant defenses of the brain, contributing to cognitive impairment [78]. On balance, the literature supports the notion that circadian disruption due to SW plays a critical role in cognitive functions. However, additional studies are needed to confirm the association between SW and cognitive impairment, as well as any physiological pathways that underlie the mechanism.

Results of the multivariate analysis also revealed substantial associations between socio-demographic factors among shift workers and cognitive impairment (S2 Appendix). Consistent with literature [79, 80] non-white ethnicity in our study sample was significantly associated with cognitive impairment. Similarly, workers who were current smokers [81], have depression [82] and have higher BMI were associated with higher odds of cognitive impairment. An explanation for these findings is that the smoking can cause periventricular and subcortical white matter lesion progression [83], cholinergic system in the basal forebrain can be effected by depression [84], and obesity can cause local inflammation within the hypothalamus that alters synaptic plasticity, thus contributing to neurodegeneration [85]. In addition, high income groups and better social support were related to reduce cognitive impairment. Employment and income levels are indicators of economic security as well as social and psychological stress, which can affect brain function and cognition [5, 86, 87]. However, lower odds of cognitive impairment among older age groups in our study is contrary to what was previously reported [35, 37, 88]. This inconsistency may be due to exclusion of persons with overt cognitive impairment from CLSA database at baseline, as a result cognitively healthy subgroup of the population may have chosen to participate. This is probably why the overall proportion of participants with cognitive impairment decreases as age group increases.

There are some additional occupational characteristics (not included in this study) that have been linked to cognitive impairment. According to prior studies [89, 90], high mental demands at work are significantly associated with better cognitive functioning in old age. Despite the possibility that work-related stress brought on by complex work, such as inadequate job control, high job demands, a lack of social support, and manual labour, may raise the risk of dementia [91, 92], research revealed that high work complexity was associated with a lower risk of dementia [93, 94]. Cognitive reserve in workers with higher work complexity can serve as a neuroprotective agent thus postponing the cognitive decline [95]. There is an increased risk of cognitive impairment due to certain occupational and environmental exposures that are neurotoxic to brain cells, such as lead [96], organophosphate pesticides [97], and magnetic fields among electronic workers [98].

Our study had several strengths. A major strength of this study is that, to our knowledge, it is the first study to investigate the associations between different types of SW exposure (night and rotating shift), and cognitive impairment based on Canadian standards, which means that cognitive impairment was identified after comparisons with neuro-healthy normative data [48, 49]. All cognitive test scores are normalized for the participant’s age, sex, education level and language of test administration (English and French) [48]. Such normalization of cognitive scores and comparisons with normative data were lacking in a previous study [36], and are required to determine whether a person’s performance falls within the range of healthy cognitive performance [48]. In addition, a large population based sample was used involving a wide range of participants. Nonetheless, there are limitations to this study that are worth noting. There were some differences in mode of data collection between tracking and comprehensive cohorts that is phone vs. in-person respectively. We controlled for type of study cohort in all adjusted models as this approach has been previously utilized [59], reducing the potential for this to have affected the study findings. Due to the number of events evening and night shifts were pooled together as previously done by some researchers [8, 11, 99]. Some SW related information were not included, as they were not recorded in the CLSA questionnaire, such as the type and direction of rotating shifts, number of consecutive night shifts worked, and the number of days off between shifts [26]. Type and duration of jobs were not examined and the association between SW and cognitive functions may not be constant across all types and duration of jobs [100, 101]. The lack of this information is a limitation and suggest potential areas for future investigation. Some participants were excluded from analysis due to missing information related to SW schedules (N = 1,682; 3.2%) and cognitive impairment (N = 1,201; 2.3%). Despite the relatively small proportion of missing data, there were some statistically significant differences between the missing and complete cases (S1 Appendix), which may result in potential bias. Respondents were free of overt cognitive impairment at baseline and are more likely healthier than the regular population possibly leading to an underestimation of the magnitude of some of our findings. In addition, generalizability of the results is limited to those healthier than the overall population. Moreover, due to the cross-sectional nature of our study, we are unable to assess temporality in the relationship between SW and cognitive impairment, raising the possibility of reverse causation.

Conclusion

These findings highlight the negative impact of SW on cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults. By taking this modifiable risk factor into account we may enable workers to reduce cognitive impairment both during their working lives and after retirement, and support "active aging" of the workforce. Although these findings are preliminary, they suggest that SW exposure and circadian disruption may be an important factor in the risk of cognitive impairment and warrants further investigation.

Supporting information

S1 Checklist. STROBE statement—checklist of items that should be included in reports of observational studies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289718.s001

(DOCX)

S1 Appendix. Comparisons of complete versus missing cases.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289718.s002

(DOCX)

S2 Appendix. Adjusted logistic regression models with estimates for all covariates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289718.s003

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

This research was made possible using the data/biospecimens collected by the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). This research has been conducted using the CLSA dataset Baseline Tracking Dataset version 4.0, Baseline Comprehensive Dataset version 7.0, under Application Number 2010006. The CLSA is led by Drs. Parminder Raina, Christina Wolfson and Susan Kirkland.

References

  1. 1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [Internet]. 5th; Text ed. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2022. Available from: https://dsm.psychiatryonline.org/doi/book/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787
  2. 2. Tucker-Drob EM. Neurocognitive functions and everyday functions change together in old age. Neuropsychology [Internet]. 2011 May;25(3):368–77. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0022348 pmid:21417532
  3. 3. Leso V, Fontana L, Caturano A, Vetrani I, Fedele M, Iavicoli I. Impact of Shift Work and Long Working Hours on Worker Cognitive Functions: Current Evidence and Future Research Needs. Int J Environ Res Public Health [Internet]. 2021 Jun 17;18(12):6540. Available from: https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126540
  4. 4. Beydoun MA, Beydoun HA, Gamaldo AA, Teel A, Zonderman AB, Wang Y. Epidemiologic studies of modifiable factors associated with cognition and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health [Internet]. 2014 Jun 24;14(1):643. Available from: http://www.embase.com/search/results?subaction=viewrecord&from=export&id=L605313952%5Cnhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-643 pmid:24962204
  5. 5. Oremus M, Konnert C, Law J, Maxwell CJ, O’Connell ME, Tyas SL. Social support and cognitive function in middle- and older-aged adults: descriptive analysis of CLSA tracking data. Eur J Public Health [Internet]. 2019 Dec 1;29(6):1084–9. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/eurpub/article/29/6/1084/5424093 pmid:30932148
  6. 6. CAREX. Night Shift Work Occupational Exposures [Internet]. CAREX Canada. 2019 [cited 2020 Feb 18]. Available from: https://www.carexcanada.ca/profile/shiftwork-occupational-exposures/
  7. 7. Williams C. Work-life balance of shift workers [Internet]. Vol. 9, Perspectives on Labour and Income. Statistics Canada; 2008 [cited 2020 Feb 10]. p. 5–16. Available from: https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/en/pub/75-001-x/2008108/pdf/10677-eng.pdf?st=T6SFzI1T
  8. 8. Institute for Work & Health. Co-workers play an important, but sometimes “invisible” role in RTW [Internet]. 2010 [cited 2020 Mar 25]. p. 1,8. Available from: https://www.iwh.on.ca/sites/iwh/files/iwh/at-work/at_work_60.pdf
  9. 9. Khan D, Rotondi M, Edgell H, Tamim H. The association between shift work exposure and the variations in age at natural menopause among adult Canadian workers: results from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). Menopause [Internet]. 2022 Jul;29(7):795–804. Available from: https://journals.lww.com/10.1097/GME.0000000000001981 pmid:35324545
  10. 10. Vyas M V., Garg AX, Iansavichus A V., Costella J, Donner A, Laugsand LE, et al. Shift work and vascular events: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ [Internet]. 2012 Jul 26;345(jul26 1):e4800. Available from: http://www.bmj.com/cgi/doi/10.1136/bmj.e4800 pmid:22835925
  11. 11. Neil-Sztramko SE, Gotay CC, Demers PA, Campbell KL. Physical Activity, Physical Fitness, and Body Composition of Canadian Shift Workers. J Occup Environ Med. 2016;58(1):94–100.
  12. 12. Shift work and health [Internet]. Ontario Institute for Work and Health. 2012 [cited 2020 Feb 18]. Available from: https://www.iwh.on.ca/newsletters/at-work/60/shift-work-and-health-what-is-research-telling-us
  13. 13. Knutsson A, Bøggild H. Gastrointestinal disorders among shift workers. Scand J Work Environ Heal [Internet]. 2010 Mar;36(2):85–95. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20101379 pmid:20101379
  14. 14. Hansen AB, Stayner L, Hansen J, Andersen ZJ. Night shift work and incidence of diabetes in the Danish Nurse Cohort. Occup Environ Med [Internet]. 2016 Apr;73(4):262–8. Available from: http://oem.bmj.com/lookup/doi/10.1136/oemed-2015-103342 pmid:26889020
  15. 15. Rao D, Yu H, Bai Y, Zheng X, Xie L. Does night-shift work increase the risk of prostate cancer? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Onco Targets Ther [Internet]. 2015;8:2817–26. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26491356 pmid:26491356
  16. 16. Wang X, Ji A, Zhu Y, Liang Z, Wu J, Li S, et al. A meta-analysis including dose-response relationship between night shift work and the risk of colorectal cancer. Oncotarget [Internet]. 2015 Sep 22;6(28):25046–60. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26208480 pmid:26208480
  17. 17. Wegrzyn LR, Tamimi RM, Rosner BA, Brown SB, Stevens RG, Eliassen AH, et al. Rotating night-shift work and the risk of breast Cancer in the nurses’ health studies. Am J Epidemiol [Internet]. 2017 Sep 1;186(5):532–40. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28541391 pmid:28541391
  18. 18. International Agency for Research on Cancer. IARC monographs on the evaluation of carcinogenic risks to humans. Painting,Painting, firefighting, and shiftwork. Vol. 98. Lyon, France. Word Health Organization: IARC Press, International Agency for Research on Cancer; 2010.
  19. 19. Ward EM, Germolec D, Kogevinas M, McCormick D, Vermeulen R, Anisimov VN, et al. Carcinogenicity of night shift work. Lancet Oncol [Internet]. 2019 Aug;20(8):1058–9. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1470204519304553 pmid:31281097
  20. 20. Wright KP, Bogan RK, Wyatt JK. Shift work and the assessment and management of shift work disorder (SWD). Sleep Med Rev [Internet]. 2013 Feb;17(1):41–54. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22560640 pmid:22560640
  21. 21. Touitou Y, Reinberg A, Touitou D. Association between light at night, melatonin secretion, sleep deprivation, and the internal clock: Health impacts and mechanisms of circadian disruption. Life Sci [Internet]. 2017 Mar 15;173:94–106. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28214594 pmid:28214594
  22. 22. Arendt J. Shift work: Coping with the biological clock. Occup Med (Chic Ill) [Internet]. 2010 Jan 1;60(1):10–20. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/occmed/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/occmed/kqp162 pmid:20051441
  23. 23. Wright KP, Drake AL, Frey DJ, Fleshner M, Desouza CA, Gronfier C, et al. Influence of sleep deprivation and circadian misalignment on cortisol, inflammatory markers, and cytokine balance. Brain Behav Immun [Internet]. 2015 Jul;47:24–34. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25640603 pmid:25640603
  24. 24. Teclemariam-Mesbah R, Ter Horst GJ, Postema F, Wortel J, Buijs RM. Anatomical demonstration of the suprachiasmatic nucleus-pineal pathway. J Comp Neurol [Internet]. 1999 Apr 5;406(2):171–82. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10096604 pmid:10096604
  25. 25. Zeitzer JM, Dijk DJ, Kronauer RE, Brown EN, Czeisler CA. Sensitivity of the human circadian pacemaker to nocturnal light: Melatonin phase resetting and suppression. J Physiol [Internet]. 2000 Aug 1;526(3):695–702. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10922269 pmid:10922269
  26. 26. Stevens RG, Hansen J, Costa G, Haus E, Kauppinen T, Aronson KJ, et al. Considerations of circadian impact for defining “shift work” in cancer studies: IARC Working Group Report. Occup Environ Med [Internet]. 2011 Feb;68(2):154–62. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20962033 pmid:20962033
  27. 27. Arlinghaus A, Bohle P, Iskra-Golec I, Jansen N, Jay S, Rotenberg L. Working time society consensus statements: Evidence-based effects of shift work and non-standard working hours on workers, family and community. Ind Health [Internet]. 2019;57(2):184–200. Available from: https://www.jstage.jst.go.jp/article/indhealth/57/2/57_SW-4/_article pmid:30700670
  28. 28. Rajaratnam SMW, Howard ME, Grunstein RR. Sleep loss and circadian disruption in shift work: Health burden and management. Med J Aust. 2013;199(8):S11–5. pmid:24138359
  29. 29. Harrington JM. Health effects of shift work and extended hours of work. Occup Environ Med [Internet]. 2001 Jan 1;58(1):68–72. Available from: http://oem.bmj.com/content/58/1/68.abstract
  30. 30. Özdemir PG, Selvi Y, Özkol H, Aydin A, Tülüce Y, Boysan M, et al. The influence of shift work on cognitive functions and oxidative stress. Psychiatry Res [Internet]. 2013 Dec;210(3):1219–25. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0165178113005891 pmid:24176594
  31. 31. MacHi MS, Staum M, Callaway CW, Moore C, Jeong K, Suyama J, et al. The relationship between shift work, sleep, and cognition in career emergency physicians. Acad Emerg Med [Internet]. 2012 Jan;19(1):85–91. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1553-2712.2011.01254.x pmid:22221346
  32. 32. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Schneider JL, Cauley JA, Hillier TA, et al. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: The study of osteoporotic fractures. Journals Gerontol—Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci [Internet]. 2006 Apr 1;61(4):405–10. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/61.4.405 pmid:16611709
  33. 33. Rouch I, Wild P, Ansiau D, Marquié JC. Shiftwork experience, age and cognitive performance. Ergonomics [Internet]. 2005 Aug 15;48(10):1282–93. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/00140130500241670 pmid:16253945
  34. 34. Marquié JC, Tucker P, Folkard S, Gentil C, Ansiau D. Chronic effects of shift work on cognition: Findings from the VISAT longitudinal study. Occup Environ Med. 2015;72(4):258–64. pmid:25367246
  35. 35. Titova OE, Lindberg E, Elmståhl S, Lind L, Schiöth HB, Benedict C. Association between shift work history and performance on the trail making test in middle-aged and elderly humans: The EpiHealth study. Neurobiol Aging [Internet]. 2016 Sep;45:23–9. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0197458016300690 pmid:27459922
  36. 36. Alonzo R, Anderson KK, Rodrigues R, Klar N, Chiodini P, Montero-Odasso M, et al. Does Shiftwork Impact Cognitive Performance? Findings from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). Int J Environ Res Public Health [Internet]. 2022 Aug 16;19(16):10124. Available from: https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/19/16/10124 pmid:36011754
  37. 37. Bokenberger K, Ström P, Dahl Aslan AK, Ã…kerstedt T, Pedersen NL. Shift work and cognitive aging: a longitudinal study. Scand J Work Environ Health [Internet]. 2017 Sep;43(5):485–93. Available from: http://www.sjweh.fi/show_abstract.php?abstract_id=3638 pmid:28362457
  38. 38. Devore EE, Grodstein F, Schernhammer ES. Shift work and cognition in the nurses’ health study. Am J Epidemiol [Internet]. 2013 Oct 15;178(8):1296–300. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/aje/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/aje/kwt214 pmid:24076971
  39. 39. Tuokko H. The Development of Normative Data and Comparison Standards for the Cognition Measures Employed in the CLSA. CLSA webinar Ser [Internet]. 2018; Available from: https://www.clsa-elcv.ca/sites/default/files/presentations/clsa_webinar_-_january_2018_-_h_tuokko.pdf
  40. 40. Tuokko HA, Smart CM. Neuropsychology of cognitive decline: A developmental approach to assessment and intervention [Internet]. Neuropsychology of cognitive decline: A developmental approach to assessment and intervention. Guilford Publications; 2018. 387-Chapter xi, 387 Pages p. Available from: https://login.bucm.idm.oclc.org/login?url= https://www.proquest.com/books/neuropsychology-cognitive-decline-developmental/docview/2071941583/se-2?accountid=14514%0Ahttps://ucm.on.worldcat.org/atoztitles/link?sid=ProQ:&issn=&volume=&issue=&title=Neuropsycho
  41. 41. Wright KP, Lowry CA, Lebourgeois MK. Circadian and wakefulness-sleep modulation of cognition in humans. Front Mol Neurosci [Internet]. 2012;5(APRIL):50. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=PMC3328852 pmid:22529774
  42. 42. Wong IS, Smith PM, Ibrahim S, Mustard CA, Gignac MAM. Mediating pathways and gender differences between shift work and subjective cognitive function. Occup Environ Med [Internet]. 2016 Jul 15;73(11):oemed-2016–103774. Available from: https://oem.bmj.com/lookup/doi/10.1136/oemed-2016-103774 pmid:27421746
  43. 43. Raina PS, Wolfson C, Kirkland SA, Griffith LE, Oremus M, Patterson C, et al. The Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). Can J Aging / La Rev Can du Vieil [Internet]. 2009 Sep 1;28(3):221–9. Available from: https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0714980809990055/type/journal_article
  44. 44. Raina P, Wolfson C, Kirkland S, Griffith LE, Balion C, Cossette B, et al. Cohort Profile: The Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). Int J Epidemiol [Internet]. 2019 Dec 1;48(6):1752–1753j. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/ije/article/48/6/1752/5563992 pmid:31633757
  45. 45. Boyle PA, Malloy PF, Salloway S, Cahn-Weiner DA, Cohen R, Cummings JL. Executive dysfunction and apathy, predict functional impairment in Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2003;11(2):214–21. pmid:12611751
  46. 46. Ascher-Svanum H, Chen YF, Hake A, Kahle-Wrobleski K, Schuster D, Kendall D, et al. Cognitive and Functional Decline in Patients with Mild Alzheimer Dementia with or Without Comorbid Diabetes. Clin Ther. 2015;37(6):1195–205. pmid:25676448
  47. 47. Verlinden VJA, Van Der Geest JN, De Bruijn RFAG, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Ikram MA. Trajectories of decline in cognition and daily functioning in preclinical dementia. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2016;12(2):144–53. pmid:26362597
  48. 48. O’Connell ME, Kadlec H, Griffith LE, Maimon G, Wolfson C, Taler V, et al. Methodological considerations when establishing reliable and valid normative data: Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA) neuropsychological battery. Clin Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2022 Nov 17;36(8):2168–87. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/13854046.2021.1954243 pmid:34470568
  49. 49. O’connell ME, Kadlec H, Grith LE, Wolfson C, Maimon G, Taler V, et al. Cognitive Impairment Indicator for the Neuropsychological Test Batteries in the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging: Denition and Evidence for Validity. 2022;1–24. Available from: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-1353218/v1
  50. 50. Tuokko H, Griffith LE, Simard M, Taler V. Cognitive measures in the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Clin Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2017 Jan 2;31(1):233–50. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2016.1254279 pmid:27830627
  51. 51. Tuokko H, Griffith LE, Simard M, Taler V, O’Connell ME, Voll S, et al. The Canadian longitudinal study on aging as a platform for exploring cognition in an aging population. Clin Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2020;34(1):174–203. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2018.1551575 pmid:30638131
  52. 52. Derived Variables–Cognition (COG) Normative Data (Tracking Assessment) Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA),Data Support Documentation [Internet]. 2022. Available from: https://www.clsa-elcv.ca/doc/4749
  53. 53. Derived Variables–Cognition (COG) Normative Data (Comprehensive Assessment) Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA),Data Support Documentation [Internet]. 2022. Available from: https://www.clsa-elcv.ca/doc/4748
  54. 54. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH, Gault CB. Estimating the percentage of the population with abnormally low scores (or abnormally large score differences) on standardized neuropsychological test batteries: A generic method with applications. Neuropsychology [Internet]. 2007 Jul;21(4):419–30. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0894-4105.21.4.419 pmid:17605575
  55. 55. Liu J, Son S, Mcintyre J, Narushima M. Depression and cardiovascular diseases among Canadian older adults: a cross-sectional analysis of baseline data from the CLSA Comprehensive Cohort. J Geriatr Cardiol [Internet]. 2019 Dec;16(12):847–54. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31911789 pmid:31911789
  56. 56. Björgvinsson T, Kertz SJ, Bigda-Peyton JS, McCoy KL, Aderka IM. Psychometric Properties of the CES-D-10 in a Psychiatric Sample. Assessment [Internet]. 2013 Aug 18;20(4):429–36. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1073191113481998 pmid:23513010
  57. 57. Roberts KC, Rao DP, Bennett TL, Loukine L, Jayaraman GC. Prevalence and patterns of chronic disease multimorbidity and associated determinants in Canada. Heal Promot Chronic Dis Prev Canada [Internet]. 2015 Aug;35(6):87–94. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26302227 pmid:26302227
  58. 58. Sherbourne CD, Stewart AL. The MOS social support survey. Soc Sci Med [Internet]. 1991 Jan;32(6):705–14. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2035047 pmid:2035047
  59. 59. Stinchcombe A, Hammond NG. Correlates of Memory and Executive Function in Middle-Aged and Older Adults in the CLSA: A Minority Stress Approach. Journals Gerontol—Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2022;77(6):1105–17. pmid:33964152
  60. 60. Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Sampling and Computation of Response Rates and Sample Weights for the Tracking (Telephone Interview) Participants and Comprehensive Participants [Internet]. 2017 [cited 2022 May 15]. Available from: https://www.clsa-elcv.ca/doc/1041
  61. 61. Cassou B, Mandereau L, Aegerter P, Touranchet A, Derriennic F. Work-related factors associated with age at natural menopause in a generation of French gainfully employed women. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;166(4):429–38. pmid:17557899
  62. 62. Arafa A, Eshak ES, Iso H, Muraki I, Tamakoshi A. Night Work, Rotating Shift Work, and the Risk of Cancer in Japanese Men and Women: The JACC Study. J Epidemiol [Internet]. 2020;1–8. Available from: https://www.jstage.jst.go.jp/article/jea/advpub/0/advpub_JE20200208/_article
  63. 63. Szosland D. Shift work and metabolic syndrome, diabetes mellitus and ischaemic heart disease. Int J Occup Med Environ Health. 2010;23(3):287–91. pmid:20934953
  64. 64. Hänninen T, Hallikainen M, Tuomainen S, Vanhanen M, Soininen H. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment: A population-based study in elderly subjects. Acta Neurol Scand [Internet]. 2002 Sep 1;106(3):148–54. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0404.2002.01225.x pmid:12174174
  65. 65. Larrieu S, Letenneur L, Orgogozo JM, Fabrigoule C, Amieva H, Le Carret N, et al. Incidence and outcome of mild cognitive impairment in a population-based prospective cohort. Neurology [Internet]. 2002 Nov 26;59(10):1594–9. Available from: https://www.neurology.org/lookup/doi/10.1212/01.WNL.0000034176.07159.F8 pmid:12451203
  66. 66. Chireh B, D’Arcy C. A comparison of the prevalence of and modifiable risk factors for cognitive impairment among community-dwelling Canadian seniors over two decades, 1991–2009. PLoS One [Internet]. 2020;15(12 December):1–20. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242911 pmid:33326422
  67. 67. Mejía-Arango S, Miguel-Jaimes A, Villa A, Ruiz-Arregui L, Gutiérrez-Robledo LM. [Cognitive impairment and associated factors in older adults in Mexico]. Salud Publica Mex [Internet]. 2007;49 Suppl 4(SUPPL. 4):S475–81. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17724520
  68. 68. Langa KM, Larson EB, Karlawish JH, Cutler DM, Kabeto MU, Kim SY, et al. Trends in the prevalence and mortality of cognitive impairment in the United States: Is there evidence of a compression of cognitive morbidity? Alzheimer’s Dement [Internet]. 2008 Mar 27;4(2):134–44. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1016/j.jalz.2008.01.001 pmid:18631957
  69. 69. O’Connell ME, Tuokko H, Voll S, Simard M, Griffith LE, Taler V, et al. An evidence-based approach to the creation of normative data: base rates of impaired scores within a brief neuropsychological battery argue for age corrections, but against corrections for medical conditions. Clin Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2017;31(6–7):1188–203. Available from: http://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2017.1349931 pmid:28679302
  70. 70. Boivin DB, Boudreau P, Kosmadopoulos A. Disturbance of the Circadian System in Shift Work and Its Health Impact. J Biol Rhythms [Internet]. 2022 Feb;37(1):3–28. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/07487304211064218 pmid:34969316
  71. 71. Åkerstedt T. Shift work and disturbed sleep/wakefulness. Occup Med (Chic Ill) [Internet]. 2003 Mar 1;53(2):89–94. Available from: pmid:12637592
  72. 72. Leso V, Caturano A, Vetrani I, Iavicoli I. Shift or night shift work and dementia risk: A systematic review. Eur Rev Med Pharmacol Sci [Internet]. 2021 Jan;25(1):222–32. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33506911 pmid:33506911
  73. 73. Åkerstedt T, Nordin M, Alfredsson L, Westerholm P, Kecklund G. Sleep and sleepiness: Impact of entering or leaving shiftwork—A prospective study. Chronobiol Int [Internet]. 2010 Jun 19;27(5):987–96. Available from: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.3109/07420528.2010.489423 pmid:20636211
  74. 74. Härmä MI, Ilmarinen JE. Towards the 24-hour society—New approaches for aging shift workers? Scand J Work Environ Heal [Internet]. 1999 Dec;25(6):610–5. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10884161 pmid:10884161
  75. 75. Cho K, Ennaceur A, Cole JC, Suh CK. Chronic jet lag produces cognitive deficits. J Neurosci [Internet]. 2000 Mar 15;20(6):RC66–RC66. Available from: http://www.jneurosci.org/lookup/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-j0005.2000 pmid:10704520
  76. 76. Kondratova AA, Kondratov R V. The circadian clock and pathology of the ageing brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(5):325–35. pmid:22395806
  77. 77. Wulff K, Gatti S, Wettstein JG, Foster RG. Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in psychiatric and neurodegenerative disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(8):589–99. pmid:20631712
  78. 78. Srinivasan V, Pandi-Perumal SR, Maestroni GJM, Esquifino AI, Hardeland R, Cardinali DP. Role of melatonin in neurodegenerative diseases. Neurotox Res [Internet]. 2005;7(4):293–318. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03033887 pmid:16179266
  79. 79. Lee HB, Richardson AK, Black BS, Shore AD, Kasper JD, Rabins P V. Race and cognitive decline among community-dwelling elders with mild cognitive impairment: Findings from the Memory and Medical Care Study. Aging Ment Health [Internet]. 2012 Apr 1;16(3):372–7. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/13607863.2011.609533 pmid:21999809
  80. 80. Schwartz BS, Glass TA, Bolla KI, Stewart WF, Glass G, Rasmussen M, et al. Disparities in cognitive functioning by race/ethnicity in the Baltimore Memory Study. Environ Health Perspect [Internet]. 2004 Mar;112(3):314–20. Available from: https://ehp.niehs.nih.gov/doi/10.1289/ehp.6727 pmid:14998746
  81. 81. Richards M, Jarvis MJ, Thompson N, Wadsworth MEJ. Cigarette Smoking and Cognitive Decline in Midlife: Evidence from a Prospective Birth Cohort Study. Am J Public Health [Internet]. 2003 Jun;93(6):994–8. Available from: https://ajph.aphapublications.org/doi/full/10.2105/AJPH.93.6.994 pmid:12773367
  82. 82. Liew TM. Depression, subjective cognitive decline, and the risk of neurocognitive disorders. Alzheimer’s Res Ther [Internet]. 2019;11(1):70. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-019-0527-7 pmid:31399132
  83. 83. Van Dijk EJ, Prins ND, Vrooman HA, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Breteler MMB. Progression of cerebral small vessel disease in relation to risk factors and cognitive consequences: Rotterdam scan study. Stroke. 2008;39(10):2712–9. pmid:18635849
  84. 84. Scheef L, Grothe MJ, Koppara A, Daamen M, Boecker H, Biersack H, et al. Subregional volume reduction of the cholinergic forebrain in subjective cognitive decline (SCD). NeuroImage Clin [Internet]. 2019;21:101612. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S2213158218303656 pmid:30555006
  85. 85. Miller AA, Spencer SJ. Obesity and neuroinflammation: A pathway to cognitive impairment. Brain Behav Immun [Internet]. 2014 Nov;42:10–21. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0889159114000889 pmid:24727365
  86. 86. Okamoto S. Socioeconomic factors and the risk of cognitive decline among the elderly population in Japan. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry [Internet]. 2019 Feb 19;34(2):265–71. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/gps.5015 pmid:30370551
  87. 87. Koster A, Penninx BWJH, Bosma H, Kempen GIJM, Newman AB, Rubin SM, et al. Socioeconomic differences in cognitive decline and the role of biomedical factors. Ann Epidemiol [Internet]. 2005;15(8):564–71. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S104727970500061X pmid:15922627
  88. 88. Murman D. The Impact of Age on Cognition. Semin Hear [Internet]. 2015 Jul 9;36(03):111–21. Available from: http://www.thieme-connect.de/DOI/DOI?10.1055/s-0035-1555115 pmid:27516712
  89. 89. Smart EL, Gow AJ, Deary IJ. Occupational complexity and lifetime cognitive abilities. Neurology [Internet]. 2014 Dec 9;83(24):2285–91. Available from: https://www.neurology.org/lookup/doi/10.1212/WNL.0000000000001075 pmid:25411439
  90. 90. Then FS, Luck T, Heser K, Ernst A, Posselt T, Wiese B, et al. Which types of mental work demands may be associated with reduced risk of dementia? Alzheimer’s Dement [Internet]. 2017 Apr 28;13(4):431–40. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1016/j.jalz.2016.08.008
  91. 91. Andel R, Crowe M, Hahn EA, Mortimer JA, Pedersen NL, Fratiglioni L, et al. Work-Related Stress May Increase the Risk of Vascular Dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc [Internet]. 2012 Jan;60(1):60–7. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03777.x pmid:22175444
  92. 92. Marengoni A, Fratiglioni L, Bandinelli S, Ferrucci L. Socioeconomic Status During Lifetime and Cognitive Impairment No-Dementia in Late Life: The Population-Based Aging in the Chianti Area (InCHIANTI) Study. J Alzheimer’s Dis [Internet]. 2011 May 10;24(3):559–68. Available from: https://www.medra.org/servlet/aliasResolver?alias=iospress&doi=10.3233/JAD-2011-101863 pmid:21297261
  93. 93. Kröger E, Andel R, Lindsay J, Benounissa Z, Verreault R, Laurin D. Is complexity of work associated with risk of dementia? Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167(7):820–30.
  94. 94. Huang LY, Hu HY, Wang ZT, Ma YH, Dong Q, Tan L, et al. Association of Occupational Factors and Dementia or Cognitive Impairment: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Zhu LQ, editor. J Alzheimer’s Dis [Internet]. 2020 Oct 27;78(1):217–27. Available from: https://www.medra.org/servlet/aliasResolver?alias=iospress&doi=10.3233/JAD-200605
  95. 95. Bosma H, Van Boxtel MPJ, Ponds RWHM, Houx PJ, Burdorf A, Jolles J. Mental work demands protect against cognitive impairment: MAAS prospective cohort study. Exp Aging Res [Internet]. 2003 Jan 29;29(1):33–45. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/03610730303710 pmid:12735080
  96. 96. Shih RA, Glass TA, Bandeen-Roche K, Carlson MC, Bolla KI, Todd AC, et al. Environmental lead exposure and cognitive function in community-dwelling older adults. Neurology [Internet]. 2006 Nov 14;67(9):1556–62. Available from: http://n.neurology.org/content/67/9/1556.abstract pmid:16971698
  97. 97. Sarailoo M, Afshari S, Asghariazar V, Safarzadeh E, Dadkhah M. Cognitive Impairment and Neurodegenerative Diseases Development Associated with Organophosphate Pesticides Exposure: a Review Study. Neurotox Res [Internet]. 2022;40(5):1624–43. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12640-022-00552-0 pmid:36066747
  98. 98. Feychting M, Jonsson F, Pedersen NL, Ahlbom A. Occupational Magnetic Field Exposure and Neurodegenerative Disease. Epidemiology [Internet]. 2003 Jul;14(4):413–9. Available from: http://journals.lww.com/00001648-200307000-00007 pmid:12843764
  99. 99. Wong IS, McLeod CB, Demers PA. Shift work trends and risk of work injury among Canadian workers. Scand J Work Environ Heal [Internet]. 2011 Jan;37(1):54–61. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20890587 pmid:20890587
  100. 100. Andel R, Crowe M, Kareholt I, Wastesson J, Parker MG. Indicators of job strain at midlife and cognitive functioning in advanced old age. Journals Gerontol—Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci [Internet]. 2011 May 1;66 B(3):287–91. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbq105 pmid:21292810
  101. 101. Then FS, Luck T, Luppa M, Thinschmidt M, Deckert S, Nieuwenhuijsen K, et al. Systematic review of the effect of the psychosocial working environment on cognition and dementia. Occup Environ Med [Internet]. 2014 May 1;71(5):358–65. Available from: http://oem.bmj.com/content/71/5/358.abstract pmid:24259677